Refutations of Equivocal Claims: No Evidence for an Ironic Effect of Counterargument Number

被引:15
作者
Ecker, Ullrich K. H. [1 ]
Lewandowsky, Stephan [2 ,3 ]
Jayawardana, Kalpana [1 ]
Mladenovic, Alexander [1 ]
机构
[1] Univ Western Australia, Sch Psychol Sci, Perth, WA, Australia
[2] Univ Bristol, Sch Expt Psychol, Bristol, Avon, England
[3] Univ Western Australia, Perth, WA, Australia
基金
澳大利亚研究理事会;
关键词
Misinformation; Debunking; Belief updating; Refutations; CONTINUED INFLUENCE; CONCEPTUAL CHANGE; MISINFORMATION; SCIENCE; INFORMATION; RESPONSES; MEMORY; ARGUMENTATION; COMMUNICATION; POLARIZATION;
D O I
10.1016/j.jarmac.2018.07.005
中图分类号
B84 [心理学];
学科分类号
04 ; 0402 ;
摘要
This study investigated the refutation of equivocal claims using counterarguments. Common sense suggests that more counterarguments should be more effective at inducing belief change. However, some researchers have argued that in persuasive reasoning, using too many arguments might lead to counterproductive skepticism and reactance. Thus, there have been calls to actively curtail the number of counterarguments used in refutations to avoid risking an "overkill backfire effect"-an ironic strengthening of beliefs from too many counterarguments. In three experiments, we tested whether calls to limit the number of counterarguments are justified. We found that a larger number of counterarguments (between four and six) led to as much or more belief reduction compared to a smaller number of (two) counterarguments. This was not merely an effect arising from a simple numerosity heuristic, as counterarguments had to be relevant to affect beliefs: irrelevant counterarguments failed to reduce beliefs even though perceived as moderately persuasive.
引用
收藏
页码:98 / 107
页数:10
相关论文
共 47 条
  • [1] [Anonymous], 2016, POLIT BEHAV
  • [2] The effect of ad hominem attacks on the evaluation of claims promoted by scientists
    Barnes, Ralph M.
    Johnston, Heather M.
    MacKenzie, Noah
    Tobin, Stephanie J.
    Taglang, Chelsea M.
    [J]. PLOS ONE, 2018, 13 (01):
  • [3] Blake J., 1999, Local Environment, V4, P257, DOI [10.1080/13549839908725599, DOI 10.1080/13549839908725599]
  • [4] Environmental communication and the cultural politics of environmental citizenship
    Burgess, J
    Harrison, CM
    Filius, P
    [J]. ENVIRONMENT AND PLANNING A-ECONOMY AND SPACE, 1998, 30 (08): : 1445 - 1460
  • [5] INTERPERSONAL COMPARISON VERSUS PERSUASIVE ARGUMENTATION - MORE DIRECT TEST OF ALTERNATIVE EXPLANATIONS FOR GROUP-INDUCED SHIFTS IN INDIVIDUAL CHOICE
    BURNSTEIN, E
    VINOKUR, A
    TROPE, Y
    [J]. JOURNAL OF EXPERIMENTAL SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY, 1973, 9 (03) : 236 - 245
  • [6] MEANINGFUL BRANDS FROM MEANINGLESS DIFFERENTIATION - THE DEPENDENCE ON IRRELEVANT ATTRIBUTES
    CARPENTER, GS
    GLAZER, R
    NAKAMOTO, K
    [J]. JOURNAL OF MARKETING RESEARCH, 1994, 31 (03) : 339 - 350
  • [7] The search for simplicity: A fundamental cognitive principle?
    Chater, N
    [J]. QUARTERLY JOURNAL OF EXPERIMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY SECTION A-HUMAN EXPERIMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY, 1999, 52 (02): : 273 - 302
  • [8] Cook J., 2011, The debunking handbook
  • [9] Rational Irrationality: Modeling Climate Change Belief Polarization Using Bayesian Networks
    Cook, John
    Lewandowsky, Stephan
    [J]. TOPICS IN COGNITIVE SCIENCE, 2016, 8 (01) : 160 - 179
  • [10] Spare the details, share the relevance: The dilution effect in communications about carbon dioxide capture and storage
    de Vries, Gerdien
    Terwel, Bart W.
    Ellemers, Naomi
    [J]. JOURNAL OF ENVIRONMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY, 2014, 38 : 116 - 123