What contributes to false belief understanding in childhood? A multidimensional approach

被引:3
作者
Aubuchon, Olivier [1 ,2 ]
Libenstein, Jamie [1 ,2 ]
Moenner, Marina [1 ,2 ]
Seguin, Marilou [1 ,2 ]
Bellerose, Jenny [1 ]
Bernier, Annie [1 ]
Beauchamp, Miriam H. [1 ,2 ,3 ]
机构
[1] Univ Montreal, Dept psychol, Montreal, PQ, Canada
[2] Sainte Justine Hosp Res Ctr, Montreal, PQ, Canada
[3] Univ Montreal, Dept Psychol, CP 6128 Succursale Ctr Ville, Montreal, PQ H3C 3J7, Canada
关键词
Theory of mind; False belief understanding; Temperament; Executive functions; Vocabulary; Parent -child interaction; Attachment; Siblings; MUTUALLY RESPONSIVE ORIENTATION; MATERNAL MIND-MINDEDNESS; EXECUTIVE FUNCTION; INDIVIDUAL-DIFFERENCES; PRESCHOOL-CHILDREN; EARLY MANIFESTATIONS; ATTACHMENT SECURITY; MOTHER-CHILD; TEMPERAMENT; LANGUAGE;
D O I
10.1016/j.cogdev.2023.101382
中图分类号
B844 [发展心理学(人类心理学)];
学科分类号
040202 ;
摘要
False belief understanding (FBU), a core component of Theory of Mind (ToM), refers to the capacity to understand that other individuals act according to their beliefs even when those beliefs are inaccurate. FBU is an important aspect of socio-cognitive development in early childhood. A range of sociodemographic, temperamental, cognitive, and family factors are known to contribute separately to individual differences in FBU; however, these domains are typically studied in silos. This longitudinal study aimed to examine which of these factors are associated with FBU around the time of school entry. We evaluated 102 typically developing children at 2-5.5 years (predictive factors) and then at 4-7.5 years of age (FBU). The findings indicate that cognitive and family factors during the preschool years are associated with FBU at school entry. Specifically, child vocabulary and parent-child interaction quality were significant independent predictors of FBU.
引用
收藏
页数:12
相关论文
共 86 条
[1]   Mutually responsive orientation between parents and their young children: Toward methodological advances in the science of relationships [J].
Aksan, Nazan ;
Kochanska, Grazyna ;
Ortmann, Margaret R. .
DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY, 2006, 42 (05) :833-848
[2]   Evaluating associations between parental mind-mindedness and children's developmental capacities through meta-analysis [J].
Aldrich, Naomi J. ;
Chen, Jing ;
Alfieri, Louis .
DEVELOPMENTAL REVIEW, 2021, 60
[3]  
Bardikoff N, 2017, NEW PERSPECTIVES ON HUMAN DEVELOPMENT, P47
[4]   DOES THE AUTISTIC-CHILD HAVE A THEORY OF MIND [J].
BARONCOHEN, S ;
LESLIE, AM ;
FRITH, U .
COGNITION, 1985, 21 (01) :37-46
[5]  
Bayley N., 2006, The Bayley Scales of Infant and Toddler Development (BSIDIII)
[6]   Should Young Children with Traumatic Brain Injury Be Compared with Community or Orthopedic Control Participants? [J].
Beauchamp, Miriam H. ;
Landry-Roy, Catherine ;
Gravel, Jocelyn ;
Beaudoin, Cindy ;
Bernier, Annie .
JOURNAL OF NEUROTRAUMA, 2017, 34 (17) :2545-2552
[7]   SOCIAL: An Integrative Framework for the Development of Social Skills [J].
Beauchamp, Miriam H. ;
Anderson, Vicki .
PSYCHOLOGICAL BULLETIN, 2010, 136 (01) :39-64
[8]   Executive Function in Preschool Children: Test-Retest Reliability [J].
Beck, Danielle M. ;
Schaefer, Catherine ;
Pang, Karen ;
Carlson, Stephanie M. .
JOURNAL OF COGNITION AND DEVELOPMENT, 2011, 12 (02) :169-193
[9]   Understanding children's and adults' limitations in mental state reasoning [J].
Birch, SAJ ;
Bloom, P .
TRENDS IN COGNITIVE SCIENCES, 2004, 8 (06) :255-260
[10]   The development of theory of mind: predictors and moderators of improvement in kindergarten [J].
Brock, Laura L. ;
Kim, Helyn ;
Gutshall, Claire C. ;
Grissmer, David W. .
EARLY CHILD DEVELOPMENT AND CARE, 2019, 189 (12) :1914-1924