Cognitive training of mice attenuates age-related decline in associative learning and behavioral flexibility

被引:3
|
作者
Attalla, Dalia [1 ]
Schatz, Alexej [1 ]
Stumpenhorst, Katharina [1 ]
Winter, York [1 ]
机构
[1] Humboldt Univ, Inst Biol, Fac Life Sci, Neurobiol Dept, Berlin, Germany
来源
FRONTIERS IN BEHAVIORAL NEUROSCIENCE | 2024年 / 18卷
关键词
touchscreen operant chamber; ID-based sorter; home-cage-based testing; spatial working memory; attention; reversal learning; TOUCHSCREEN OPERANT PLATFORM; REACTION-TIME-TASK; MOUSE MODELS; HOME-CAGE; MEMORY; RATS; DISCRIMINATION; REVERSAL; SENSITIVITY; ATTENTION;
D O I
10.3389/fnbeh.2024.1326501
中图分类号
B84 [心理学]; C [社会科学总论]; Q98 [人类学];
学科分类号
03 ; 0303 ; 030303 ; 04 ; 0402 ;
摘要
Identifying factors that influence age-related cognitive decline is crucial, given its severe personal and societal impacts. However, studying aging in human or animal models is challenging due to the significant variability in aging processes among individuals. Additionally, longitudinal and cross-sectional studies often produce differing results. In this context, home-cage-based behavioral analysis over lifespans has emerged as a significant method in recent years. This study aimed to explore how prior experience affects cognitive performance in mice of various age groups (4, 12, and 22 months) using a home-cage-based touchscreen test battery. In this automated system, group-housed, ID-chipped mice primarily obtain their food during task performance throughout the day, motivated by their own initiative, without being subjected to food deprivation. Spatial working memory and attention were evaluated using the trial unique non-matching to location (TUNL) and the five-choice serial reaction time task (5-CSRTT), respectively. The same set of mice learned both of these demanding tasks. While signs of cognitive decline were already apparent in middle-aged mice, older mice exhibited poorer performance in both tasks. Mice at both 12 and 22 months displayed an increase in perseverance and a decrease in the percentage of correct responses in the TUNL test compared to the 4-month-old mice. Furthermore, during the 5-CSRTT, they exhibited higher rates of omissions and premature responses compared to their younger counterparts. Additionally, the correct response rate in 22-month-old mice was lower than that of the 4-month-old ones. However, mice that had undergone cognitive training at 4 months maintained high-performance levels when re-tested at 12 months, showing an increase in correct responses during TUNL testing compared to their untrained controls. In the 5-CSRTT, previously trained mice demonstrated higher correct response rates, fewer omissions, and reduced premature responses compared to naive control mice. Notably, even when assessed on a visual discrimination and behavioral flexibility task at 22 months, experienced mice outperformed naive 4-month-old mice. These findings highlight the advantages of early-life cognitive training and suggest that its benefits extend beyond the cognitive domains primarily targeted during early training. The success of this study was significantly aided by the fully automated home-cage-based testing system, which allows for high throughput with minimal human intervention.
引用
收藏
页数:17
相关论文
共 50 条
  • [1] Progressive Age-Related Cognitive Decline in Tau Mice
    Van der Jeugd, Ann
    Vermaercke, Ben
    Derisbourg, Maxime
    Lo, Adrian C.
    Hamdane, Malika
    Blum, David
    Buee, Luc
    D'Hooge, Rudi
    JOURNAL OF ALZHEIMERS DISEASE, 2013, 37 (04) : 777 - 788
  • [2] Age-related Changes in Associative Learning for Olfactory and Visual Stimuli in Rodents
    Gilbert, Paul E.
    Pirogovsky, Eva
    Brushfield, Andrea M.
    Luu, Trinh T.
    Tolentino, Jerlyn C.
    Renteria, Adam F.
    INTERNATIONAL SYMPOSIUM ON OLFACTION AND TASTE, 2009, 1170 : 718 - 724
  • [3] Age-related decline in cognitive flexibility is associated with the levels of hippocampal neurogenesis
    Amelchenko, Evgeny M.
    Bezriadnov, Dmitri V.
    Chekhov, Olga A.
    Anokhin, Konstantin V.
    Lazutkin, Alexander A.
    Enikolopov, Grigori
    FRONTIERS IN NEUROSCIENCE, 2023, 17
  • [4] Pathways to age-related cognitive decline
    Kramer, Joel
    ANNALS OF NEUROLOGY, 2013, 73 (05) : 563 - 564
  • [5] When does age-related cognitive decline begin?
    Salthouse, Timothy A.
    NEUROBIOLOGY OF AGING, 2009, 30 (04) : 507 - 514
  • [6] Age-Related Decline in the Topological Efficiency of the Brain Structural Connectome and Cognitive Aging
    Li, Xin
    Wang, Yezhou
    Wang, Wenxiao
    Huang, Weijie
    Chen, Kewei
    Xu, Kai
    Zhang, Junying
    Chen, Yaojing
    Li, He
    Wei, Dongfeng
    Shu, Ni
    Zhang, Zhanjun
    CEREBRAL CORTEX, 2020, 30 (08) : 4651 - 4661
  • [7] Age-related decline in cognitive flexibility and inadequate preparation: evidence from task-state network analysis
    Xia, Haishuo
    Li, Ting
    Hou, Yongqing
    Liu, Zijin
    Chen, Antao
    GEROSCIENCE, 2024, 46 (06) : 5939 - 5953
  • [8] Adult Age-Related Differences in Appetitive and Aversive Associative Learning
    Freund, Alexandra M.
    Keil, Andreas
    EMOTION, 2021, 21 (06) : 1239 - 1251
  • [9] Decalepis hamiltonii root extract attenuates the age-related decline in the cognitive function in Drosophila melanogaster
    Haddadi, Mohammad
    Jahromi, Samaneh Reiszadeh
    Shivanandappa, T.
    Ramesh, S. R.
    BEHAVIOURAL BRAIN RESEARCH, 2013, 249 : 8 - 14
  • [10] Meditation and successful aging: can meditative practices counteract age-related cognitive decline?
    Sperduti, Marco
    Makowski, Dominique
    Blonde, Philippe
    Piolino, Pascale
    GERIATRIE ET PSYCHOLOGIE NEUROPSYCHIATRIE DE VIEILLISSEMENT, 2017, 15 (02): : 205 - 213