Marmoset monkeys use different avoidance strategies to cope with ambient noise during vocal behavior

被引:3
作者
Loeschner, Julia [1 ,2 ]
Pomberger, Thomas [1 ,3 ,4 ]
Hage, Steffen R. [1 ,2 ]
机构
[1] Univ Tubingen, Hearing Res Ctr, Dept Otolaryngol Head & Neck Surg, Neurobiol Social Commun,Med Ctr, Elfriede Aulhorn Str 5, D-72076 Tubingen, Germany
[2] Univ Tubingen, Werner Reichardt Ctr Integrat Neurosci, Otfried Muller Str 25, D-72076 Tubingen, Germany
[3] Univ Tubingen, Grad Sch Neural & Behav Sci, Int Max Planck Res Sch, Osterberg Str 3, D-72074 Tubingen, Germany
[4] Duke Univ, Sch Med, Dept Neurobiol, Neurobiol Commun, 311 Res Dr, Durham, NC 27710 USA
关键词
ACOUSTIC COMMUNICATION; COMMON MARMOSET; REPERTOIRE; FREQUENCY; AMPLITUDE; TAMARINS;
D O I
10.1016/j.isci.2023.106219
中图分类号
O [数理科学和化学]; P [天文学、地球科学]; Q [生物科学]; N [自然科学总论];
学科分类号
07 ; 0710 ; 09 ;
摘要
Multiple strategies have evolved to compensate for masking noise, leading to changes in call features. One call adjustment is the Lombard effect, an increase in call amplitude in response to noise. Another strategy involves call production in periods where noise is absent. While mechanisms underlying vocal adjustments have been well studied, mechanisms underlying noise avoidance strategies remain largely unclear. We systematically perturbed ongoing phee calls of mar-mosets to investigate noise avoidance strategies. Marmosets canceled their calls after noise onset and produced longer calls after noise-phases ended. Addition-ally, the number of uttered syllables decreased during noise perturbation. This behavior persisted beyond the noise-phase. Using machine learning techniques, we found that a fraction of single phees were initially planned as double phees and became interrupted after the first syllable. Our findings indicate that marmo-sets use different noise avoidance strategies and suggest vocal flexibility at different complexity levels in the marmoset brain.
引用
收藏
页数:13
相关论文
共 28 条
  • [1] Brain mechanisms of acoustic communication in humans and nonhuman primates: An evolutionary perspective
    Ackermann, Hermann
    Hage, Steffen R.
    Ziegler, Wolfram
    [J]. BEHAVIORAL AND BRAIN SCIENCES, 2014, 37 (06) : 529 - 546
  • [2] A quantitative acoustic analysis of the vocal repertoire of the common marmoset (Callithrix jacchus)
    Agamaite, James A.
    Chang, Chia-Jung
    Osmanski, Michael S.
    Wang, Xiaoqin
    [J]. JOURNAL OF THE ACOUSTICAL SOCIETY OF AMERICA, 2015, 138 (05) : 2906 - 2928
  • [3] Structure and usage of the vocal repertoire of Callithrix jacchus
    Bezerra, Bruna Martins
    Souto, Antonio
    [J]. INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL OF PRIMATOLOGY, 2008, 29 (03) : 671 - 701
  • [4] Bradbury Jack W., 1998, pi
  • [5] Low-amplitude noise elicits the Lombard effect in plainfin midshipman mating vocalizations in the wild
    Brown, Nicholas A. W.
    Halliday, William D.
    Balshine, Sigal
    Juanes, Francis
    [J]. ANIMAL BEHAVIOUR, 2021, 181 : 29 - 39
  • [6] Acoustic communication in noise
    Brumm, H
    Slabbekoorn, H
    [J]. ADVANCES IN THE STUDY OF BEHAVIOR, VOL 35, 2005, 35 : 151 - 209
  • [7] Acoustic communication in noise:: regulation of call characteristics in a New World monkey
    Brumm, H
    Voss, K
    Köllmer, I
    Todt, D
    [J]. JOURNAL OF EXPERIMENTAL BIOLOGY, 2004, 207 (03) : 443 - 448
  • [8] Vocal plasticity in a reptile
    Brumm, Henrik
    Zollinger, Sue Anne
    [J]. PROCEEDINGS OF THE ROYAL SOCIETY B-BIOLOGICAL SCIENCES, 2017, 284 (1855)
  • [9] The evolution of the Lombard effect: 100 years of psychoacoustic research
    Brumm, Henrik
    Zollinger, Sue Anne
    [J]. BEHAVIOUR, 2011, 148 (11-13) : 1173 - 1198
  • [10] Coevolution of vocal signal characteristics and hearing sensitivity in forest mammals
    Charlton, Benjamin D.
    Owen, Megan A.
    Swaisgood, Ronald R.
    [J]. NATURE COMMUNICATIONS, 2019, 10 (1)