Motion duration is overestimated behind an occluder in action and perception tasks

被引:1
作者
Menceloglu, Melisa [1 ]
Song, Joo-Hyun [1 ,2 ]
机构
[1] Brown Univ, Dept Cognit Linguist & Psychol Sci, Providence, RI 02912 USA
[2] Brown Univ, Carney Inst Brain Sci, Providence, RI USA
来源
JOURNAL OF VISION | 2023年 / 23卷 / 05期
关键词
TIME-TO-CONTACT; EYE-MOVEMENTS; PREDICTION-MOTION; SPEED PERCEPTION; EXTRAPOLATION; INTEGRATION; ATTENTION; INTERVALS; ACCURACY; MODALITY;
D O I
10.1167/jov.23.5.11
中图分类号
R77 [眼科学];
学科分类号
100212 ;
摘要
Motion estimation behind an occluder is a common task in situations like crossing the street or passing another car. People tend to overestimate the duration of an object's motion when it gets occluded for subsecond motion durations. Here, we explored (a) whether this bias depended on the type of interceptive action: discrete keypress versus continuous reach and (b) whether it was present in a perception task without an interceptive action. We used a prediction-motion task and presented a bar moving across the screen with a constant velocity that later became occluded. In the action task, participants stopped the occluded bar when they thought the bar reached the goal position via keypress or reach. They were more likely to stop the bar after it passed the goal position regardless of the action type, suggesting that the duration of occluded motion was overestimated (or its speed was underestimated). In the perception task, where participants judged whether a tone was presented before or after the bar reached the goal position, a similar bias was observed. In both tasks, the bias was near constant across motion durations and directions and grew over trials. We speculate that this robust bias may be due to a temporal illusion, Bayesian slow-motion prior, or the processing of the visible-occluded boundary crossing. Understanding its exact mechanism, the conditions on which it depends, and the relative roles of speed and time perception requires further research.
引用
收藏
页数:14
相关论文
共 10 条
  • [1] Motion behind occluder: Amodal perception and visual motion extrapolation
    Battaglini, Luca
    Ghiani, Andrea
    VISUAL COGNITION, 2021, 29 (08) : 475 - 499
  • [2] Biological motion drives perception and action
    de Xivry, Jean-Jacques Orban
    Coppe, Sebastien
    Lefevre, Philippe
    Missal, Marcus
    JOURNAL OF VISION, 2010, 10 (02): : 1 - 11
  • [3] Perception of Looming Motion in Virtual Reality Egocentric Interception Tasks
    Rolin, Robert A.
    Fooken, Jolande
    Spering, Miriam
    Pai, Dinesh K.
    IEEE TRANSACTIONS ON VISUALIZATION AND COMPUTER GRAPHICS, 2019, 25 (10) : 3042 - 3048
  • [4] Action induction by visual perception of rotational motion
    Classen, Claudia
    Kibele, Armin
    PSYCHOLOGICAL RESEARCH-PSYCHOLOGISCHE FORSCHUNG, 2016, 80 (05): : 785 - 804
  • [5] Voluntary action affects perception of bistable motion display
    Mitsumatsu, Hidemichi
    PERCEPTION, 2009, 38 (10) : 1522 - 1535
  • [6] Action induction due to visual perception of linear motion in depth
    Classen, Claudia
    Kibele, Armin
    PSYCHOLOGICAL RESEARCH-PSYCHOLOGISCHE FORSCHUNG, 2017, 81 (01): : 131 - 142
  • [7] The speed and temporal frequency of visual apparent motion modulate auditory duration perception
    He, Xiang
    Ke, Zijun
    Wu, Zehua
    Chen, Lihan
    Yue, Zhenzhu
    SCIENTIFIC REPORTS, 2023, 13 (01)
  • [8] Motion-induced illusory displacement reexamined: differences between perception and action?
    Dirk Kerzel
    Karl R. Gegenfurtner
    Experimental Brain Research, 2005, 162 : 191 - 201
  • [9] Motion-induced illusory displacement reexamined: differences between perception and action?
    Kerzel, D
    Gegenfurtner, KR
    EXPERIMENTAL BRAIN RESEARCH, 2005, 162 (02) : 191 - 201
  • [10] Audiovisual Integration During Joint Action: No Effects for Motion Discrimination and Temporal Order Judgment Tasks
    Wahn, Basil
    Dosso, Jill A.
    Kingstone, Alan
    FRONTIERS IN PSYCHOLOGY, 2020, 11