Camouflage using three-dimensional surface disruption

被引:2
作者
King, Jemma [1 ]
Hemmi, Jan M. [1 ,2 ]
Kelley, Jennifer L. [1 ,2 ]
机构
[1] Univ Western Australia, Sch Biol Sci, 35 Stirling Highway, Perth 6009, Australia
[2] Univ Western Australia, UWA Oceans Inst, 35 Stirling Highway, Perth 6009, Australia
基金
澳大利亚研究理事会;
关键词
disruptive coloration; background matching; crypsis; predation; perception; vision; COLORATION; CRYPSIS;
D O I
10.1098/rsbl.2022.0596
中图分类号
Q [生物科学];
学科分类号
07 ; 0710 ; 09 ;
摘要
Disruptive markings are common in animal patterns and can provide camouflage benefits by concealing the body's true edges and/or by breaking the surface of the body into multiple depth planes. Disruptive patterns that are accentuated by high contrast borders are most likely to provide false depth cues to enhance camouflage, but studies to date have used visual detection models or humans as predators. We presented three-dimensional-printed moth-like targets to wild bird predators to determine whether: (1) three-dimensional prey with disrupted body surfaces have higher survival than three-dimensional prey with continuous surfaces, (2) two-dimensional prey with disruptive patterns or enhanced edge markings have higher survival than non-patterned two-dimensional prey. We found a survival benefit for three-dimensional prey with disrupted surfaces, and a significant effect of mean wing luminance. There was no evidence that false depth cues provided the same protective benefits as physical surface disruption in three-dimensional prey, perhaps because our treatments did not mimic the complexity of patterns found in natural animal markings. Our findings indicate that disruption of surface continuity is an important strategy for concealing a three-dimensional body shape.
引用
收藏
页数:5
相关论文
共 50 条
  • [41] The role of color in the perception of three-dimensional shape
    Marlow, Phillip J.
    Gegenfurtner, Karl R.
    Anderson, Barton L.
    CURRENT BIOLOGY, 2022, 32 (06) : 1387 - +
  • [42] Development of Three-Dimensional Completion of Complex Objects
    Soska, Kasey C.
    Johnson, Scott P.
    INFANCY, 2013, 18 (03) : 325 - 344
  • [43] Three-dimensional coherence of the conscious body image
    Longo, Matthew R.
    QUARTERLY JOURNAL OF EXPERIMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY, 2015, 68 (06) : 1116 - 1123
  • [44] Overestimation of the number of elements in a three-dimensional stimulus
    Aida, Saori
    Kusano, Tsutomu
    Shimono, Koichi
    Tam, Wa James
    JOURNAL OF VISION, 2015, 15 (09):
  • [45] Impact of Aging on Three-Dimensional Facial Verification
    Heravi, Farnaz Majid Zadeh
    Farazdaghi, Elham
    Fournier, Regis
    Nait-ali, Amine
    ELECTRONICS, 2019, 8 (10)
  • [46] Anticipating the three-dimensional consequences of eye movements
    Wexler, M
    PROCEEDINGS OF THE NATIONAL ACADEMY OF SCIENCES OF THE UNITED STATES OF AMERICA, 2005, 102 (04) : 1246 - 1251
  • [47] Coupled computations of three-dimensional shape and material
    Marlow, Phillip J.
    Todorovic, Dejan
    Anderson, Barton L.
    CURRENT BIOLOGY, 2015, 25 (06) : R221 - R222
  • [48] Infants Perceive Three-Dimensional Subjective Contours
    Kavsek, Michael
    Braun, Stephanie
    PERCEPTION, 2018, 47 (12) : 1153 - 1165
  • [49] Two-Dimensional Versus Three-Dimensional Laparoscopy: Evaluation of Physicians' Performance and Preference Using a Pelvic Trainer
    Ko, Jennifer K. Y.
    Li, Raymond H. W.
    Cheung, Vincent Y. T.
    JOURNAL OF MINIMALLY INVASIVE GYNECOLOGY, 2015, 22 (03) : 421 - 427
  • [50] Three-dimensional reconstruction of underwater objects using wide-aperture imaging SONAR
    Guerneve, Thomas
    Subr, Kartic
    Petillot, Yvan
    JOURNAL OF FIELD ROBOTICS, 2018, 35 (06) : 890 - 905