Perception, action, and the body model

被引:5
作者
Coelho, Lara A. [1 ,2 ]
Gonzalez, Claudia L. R. [1 ]
机构
[1] Univ Lethbridge, Dept Kinesiol, Brain Act Lab, Lethbridge, AB, Canada
[2] Italian Inst Technol, UVIP Unit Visually Impaired People, Genoa, Italy
基金
加拿大自然科学与工程研究理事会;
关键词
TOOL-USE; SIZE PERCEPTION; VISUAL SIZE; REPRESENTATION; HAND; SCHEMA; ILLUSION; TACTILE; VISION; BRAIN;
D O I
10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2024.108853
中图分类号
B84 [心理学]; C [社会科学总论]; Q98 [人类学];
学科分类号
03 ; 0303 ; 030303 ; 04 ; 0402 ;
摘要
In 1992, Goodale and Milner proposed to study the visual system based on function, thus dissociating vision for perception (ventral stream) and vision for action (dorsal stream). This became known as the Perception and Action model (PAM). Following the PAM in the visual system, a somatosensory PAM was proposed including a body representation for perception and a separate for action. This review explores the body model of the hand and how it relates to the PAM. The body model refers to the internal representation of the body that is responsible for position sense. Previous research has shown that the representation of the hand features systematic distortions: an overestimation of hand width and an underestimation of finger length. These distortions have been reported using different paradigms, different body parts, and in various settings. Thus, body model distortions appear to be a characteristic of human body representation. If the body model of the hand is distorted, how can actions like reaching and grasping be accurate? We review evidence that body model distortions may in fact provide a functional benefit to our actions, that cortical maps in the somatosensory and motor cortices reflect these distortions, and that actions rely on a distorted body model. We argue that the body model is a product of both the ventral and dorsal somatosensory streams. Further, we suggest that the body model is an example of the inextricable link between the two streams.
引用
收藏
页数:7
相关论文
共 50 条
[41]   How our body influences our perception of the world [J].
Harris, Laurence R. ;
Carnevale, Michael J. ;
D'Amour, Sarah ;
Fraser, Lindsey E. ;
Harrar, Vanessa ;
Hoover, Adria E. N. ;
Mander, Charles ;
Pritchett, Lisa M. .
FRONTIERS IN PSYCHOLOGY, 2015, 6
[42]   Growing into your hand: the developmental trajectory of the body model [J].
Coelho, Lara A. ;
Gonzalez, Claudia L. R. .
EXPERIMENTAL BRAIN RESEARCH, 2022, 240 (01) :135-145
[43]   Objects exhibit body model like shape distortions [J].
Saulton, Aurelie ;
Dodds, Trevor J. ;
Buelthoff, Heinrich H. ;
de la Rosa, Stephan .
EXPERIMENTAL BRAIN RESEARCH, 2015, 233 (05) :1471-1479
[44]   Interactions of different body parts in peripersonal space: how vision of the foot influences tactile perception at the hand [J].
Schicke, Tobias ;
Bauer, Frank ;
Roder, Brigitte .
EXPERIMENTAL BRAIN RESEARCH, 2009, 192 (04) :703-715
[45]   Visuo-tactile shape perception in women with Anorexia Nervosa and healthy women with and without body concerns [J].
Risso, G. ;
Martoni, R. M. ;
Erzegovesi, S. ;
Bellodi, L. ;
Baud-Bovy, G. .
NEUROPSYCHOLOGIA, 2020, 149
[46]   Recent advances in perception and action [J].
Gegenfurtner, Karl ;
Henriques, Denise ;
Krauzlis, Rich .
VISION RESEARCH, 2011, 51 (08) :801-803
[47]   PERCEPTION IS KINAESTHESIA, THE VERB IS ACTION [J].
BARANDE, I .
REVUE FRANCAISE DE PSYCHANALYSE, 1995, 59 (02) :367-377
[48]   Action's Effect on Perception [J].
Witt, Jessica K. .
CURRENT DIRECTIONS IN PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE, 2011, 20 (03) :201-206
[49]   Reading action word affects the visual perception of biological motion [J].
Bidet-Ildei, Christel ;
Sparrow, Laurent ;
Coello, Yann .
ACTA PSYCHOLOGICA, 2011, 137 (03) :330-334
[50]   Priming of the Sander Parallelogram illusion separates perception from action [J].
Senanayake, Shannon A. ;
Carther-Krone, Tiffany ;
Marotta, Jonathan J. .
EXPERIMENTAL BRAIN RESEARCH, 2021, 239 (07) :2207-2220